https://eutils.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/eutils/efetch.fcgi?db=pubmed&id=39171231&retmode=xml&tool=Litmetric&email=readroberts32@gmail.com&api_key=61f08fa0b96a73de8c900d749fcb997acc09 3917123120240823
1664-1078152024Frontiers in psychologyFront PsycholMind wandering is not always harmful in sports: the role of its content.13488931348893134889310.3389/fpsyg.2024.1348893Mind wandering (MW) among athletes during training and competition can lead to poor performance. However, MW has also been found to have positive effects. This study aims to clarify the causes of the bidirectional (negative and positive) effects of MW in the sports context, specifically focusing on whether these effects are related to the content of MW.A total of 846 Chinese athletes completed the Chinese version of the MW scales. The survey data were tested for common method biases. Subsequently, Pearson correlation analysis and structural equation modeling were performed using SPSS 25.0 and Mplus 7.0.The frequency of MW can positively predict its bidirectional effects. MW content plays an important role in the relationships between MW frequency and its negative and positive effects, but the direction of influence varies depending on the content.MW in sports is not always harmful, and its content plays an important role. These findings suggest that managing MW content may be a promising MW intervention method for improving performance in sports.Copyright © 2024 Li, Li, Xue and He.LiJielingJPhysical Education Postdoctoral Research Station, Hebei Normal University, Shijiazhuang, China.School of Physical Education, Hebei Normal University, Shijiazhuang, China.Key Laboratory of Measurement and Evaluation in Exercise Bioinformation of Hebei Province, Shijiazhuang, China.LiChuangyeCPhysical Education Postdoctoral Research Station, Hebei Normal University, Shijiazhuang, China.XueShuangpengSPhysical Education Postdoctoral Research Station, Hebei Normal University, Shijiazhuang, China.HeYuxiuYPhysical Education Postdoctoral Research Station, Hebei Normal University, Shijiazhuang, China.School of Physical Education, Hebei Normal University, Shijiazhuang, China.Key Laboratory of Measurement and Evaluation in Exercise Bioinformation of Hebei Province, Shijiazhuang, China.engJournal Article20240807
SwitzerlandFront Psychol1015509021664-1078athletecontentfrequencymind wanderingperformanceThe authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
20231232024723202482264220248226412024822450202487epublish39171231PMC1133669710.3389/fpsyg.2024.1348893Aitken J. A., Pagan O., Wong C. M., Bayley B., Helton W. S., Kaplan S. A. (2023). Task-related and task-unrelated thoughts in runners and equestrians: measurement issues in evaluations of thought content. Appl. Ergon. 110:104011. 10.1016/j.apergo.2023.10401110.1016/j.apergo.2023.10401136905727Albert D. A., Ouimet M. C., Jarret J., Cloutier M. S., Paquette M., Badeau N., et al. . (2018). Linking mind wandering tendency to risky driving in young male drivers. Accid. Anal. Prev. 111, 125–132. 10.1016/j.aap.2017.11.01910.1016/j.aap.2017.11.01929197692Baird B., Smallwood J., Mrazek M. D., Kam J. W. Y., Franklin M. S., Schooler J. W. (2012). Inspired by distraction: mind wandering facilitates creative incubation. Psychol. Sci. 23, 1117–1122. 10.1177/095679761244602410.1177/095679761244602422941876Baird B., Smallwood J., Schooler J. W. (2011). Back to the future: autobiographical planning and the functionality of mind-wandering. Conscious. Cogn. 20, 1604–1611. 10.1016/j.concog.2011.08.00710.1016/j.concog.2011.08.00721917482Belletier C., Davranche K., Tellier I. S., Dumas F., Vidal F., Hasbroucq T., et al. . (2015). Choking under monitoring pressure: being watched by the experimenter reduces executive attention. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 22, 1410–1416. 10.3758/s13423-015-0804-910.3758/s13423-015-0804-925673216Burdett B. R. D., Charlton S. G., Starkey N. J. (2016). Not all minds wander equally: the influence of traits, states, and road environment factors on self-reported mind wandering during everyday driving. Accid. Anal. Prev. 95, 1–7. 10.1016/j.aap.2016.06.01210.1016/j.aap.2016.06.01227372440David R. T. Derek B. and Daniel S. (2015). A resource-control account of sustained attention. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 10, 82–96. 10.1177/174569161455668110.1177/174569161455668125910383Dias Da Silva M. R., Postma M. (2022). Straying off course: the negative impact of mind wandering on fine motor movements. J. Mot. Behav. 54, 186–202. 10.1080/00222895.2021.193703210.1080/00222895.2021.193703234346297Doll W. J. Xia W. and Torkzadeh G. (1994). A confirmatory factor analysis of the end user computing satisfaction instrument. Mis. Quart. 18, 453–461. 10.2307/24952410.2307/249524Gouraud J., Delorme A., Berberian B. (2018). Influence of automation on mind wandering frequency in sustained attention. Conscious. Cogn. 66, 54–64. 10.1016/j.concog.2018.09.01210.1016/j.concog.2018.09.01230396034Kane M. J., Brown L. H., McVay J. C., Silvia P. J., Myin-Germeys I., Kwapil T. R. (2007). For whom the mind wanders, and when: an experience-sampling study of working memory and executive control in daily life. Psychol. Sci. 18, 614–621. 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01948.x10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01948.x17614870Klinger E., Cox W. M. (1987). Dimensions of thought flow in everyday life. Imagin. Cogn. Pers. 7, 105–128. 10.2190/7K24-G343-MTQW-115V10.2190/7K24-G343-MTQW-115V0Kurzban R., Duckworth A., Kable J. W., Myers J. (2013). An opportunity cost model of subjective effort and task performance. Behav. Brain. Sci. 36, 661–679. 10.1017/S0140525X1200319610.1017/S0140525X12003196PMC385632024304775Latinjak A. T. (2018). Athletes' self-reports on mind wandering while practicing sports: an exploratory two-study project. J. Clin. Sports Psychol. 12, 432–447. 10.1123/jcsp.2017-002310.1123/jcsp.2017-0023Li J. L., Liu Y. F., Xue S. P., Tian B. (2024). Costs over benefits: mind wandering in sportsing performance. Front. Psychol. 2024:1347561. 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.134756110.3389/fpsyg.2024.1347561PMC1125049239015331Li J. L., Yao J. X. (2016). Athlete's mind wandering: qualitative research based on grounded theory. China Sports Sci. Technol. 52, 43–50. 10.16470/j.csst.20160600710.16470/j.csst.201606007Li J. L., Yao J. X. (2017). Development of athlete's mind wandering scale on the cause, content and results. J Tianjin Univ Sports 32, 448–454. 10.13297/j.cnki.issn1005-0000.2017.05.01210.13297/j.cnki.issn1005-0000.2017.05.012Miś M., Kowalczyk M. (2021). Mind-wandering during long-distance running and mood change. The role of working memory capacity and temporal orientation of thoughts. Int. J. Sports Exerc. Psychol. 19, 815–833. 10.1080/1612197X.2020.176653810.1080/1612197X.2020.1766538Mooneyham B. W., Schooler J. W. (2013). The costs and benefits of mind-wandering: a review. Can. J. Exp. Psychol. 67, 11–18. 10.1037/a003156910.1037/a003156923458547Netease Sports News (2011). Roger Federer: Sometimes Our Minds Wander During the Match. We're Not Robocop. Available at: http://sports.163.com/11/0224/09/6TL9TB7T00051CDG.html (accessed March 15, 2023).Podsakoff P. M., MacKenzie S. B., Lee J. Y., Podsakoff N. P. (2003). Common method biases in behavioral research: a critical review of the literature and recommended remedies. J. Appl. Psychol. 88, 879–903. 10.1037/0021-9010.88.5.87910.1037/0021-9010.88.5.87914516251Preiss D. D. (2022). Metacognition, mind wandering, and cognitive flexibility: understanding creativity. J. Intell. 10:69. 10.3390/jintelligence1003006910.3390/jintelligence10030069PMC950063436135610Salavera C., Usán P. (2020). The mediating role of affects between mind-wandering and happiness. Sustainability 12:5139. 10.3390/su1212513910.3390/su12125139Schücker L., Hagemann N., Strauss B., Völker K. (2009). The effect of attentional focus on running economy. J. Sports Sci. 27, 1241–1248. 10.1080/0264041090315046710.1080/0264041090315046719787539Schumacker R. E., Lomax R. G. (2004). A Beginner's Guide to Structural Equation Modeling, 3 Edn. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Seli P., Cheyne J. A., Xu M., Purdon C., Smilek D. (2015). Motivation, intentionality, and mind wandering: implications for assessments of task-unrelated thought. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 41, 1417–1425. 10.1037/xlm000011610.1037/xlm000011625730306Seli P., Risko E. F., Smilek D. (2016). On the necessity of distin guishing between unintentional and intentional mind wandering. Psychol. Sci. 27, 685–691. 10.1177/095679761663406810.1177/095679761663406826993740Simmonds P. J., Wakefield C. J., Coyles G., Roberts J. W. (2023). Enhanced expectancies benefit performance under distraction, but compromise it under stress: exploring the OPTIMAL theory. Hum. Mov. Sci. 89:103085. 10.1016/j.humov.2023.10308510.1016/j.humov.2023.10308537030071Smallwood J. (2010). Why the global availability of mind wandering necessitates resource competition: reply to McVay and Kane. Psychol. Bull. 136, 202–207. 10.1037/a001867310.1037/a0018673Smallwood J., Andrews-Hanna J. (2013). Not all minds that wander are lost: the importance of a balanced perspective on the mind-wandering state. Front. Psychol. 4:441. 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.0044110.3389/fpsyg.2013.00441PMC374487123966961Smallwood J., Baracaia S. F., Lowe M., Obonsawin M. (2003). Task unrelated thought whilst encoding information. Conscious. Cogn. 12, 452–484. 10.1016/S1053-8100(03)00018-710.1016/S1053-8100(03)00018-712941287Smallwood J., Mcspadden M., Schooler J. W. (2008). When attention matters: the curious incident of the wandering mind. Mem. Cogn. 36, 1144–1150. 10.3758/MC.36.6.114410.3758/MC.36.6.114418927032Smallwood J., Nind L., O'Connor R. C. (2009). When is your head at? An exploration of the factors associated with the temporal focus of the wandering mind. Conscious. Cogn. 18, 118–125. 10.1016/j.concog.2008.11.00410.1016/j.concog.2008.11.00419121953Smallwood J., Schooler J. W. (2006). The restless mind. Psychol. Bull. 132, 946–958. 10.1037/0033-2909.132.6.94610.1037/0033-2909.132.6.94617073528Sohu (2021). Emmons Hit the Wrong Target Again! Is It Still the Chinese Who Won the Championship? Did the Second Place in the Olympic Games Make a Big Mistake By Mind Wandering? Available at: https://www.sohu.com/a/497762179121048740 (accessed March 15, 2023).Teng S. C., Lien Y. W. (2022). Propensity or diversity? Investigating how mind wandering influences the incubation effect of creativity. PLoS ONE 17:e0267187. 10.1371/journal.pone.026718710.1371/journal.pone.0267187PMC905381335486594Torres-Irribarra D., Ibaceta M., Preiss D. D. (2019). Positive and negative mind wandering: an assessment of their relationship with mindfulness and metacognition in university students. Estud. Psicol. 40, 664–701. 10.1080/02109395.2019.167945710.1080/02109395.2019.1679457Welhaf M. S., Astacio M. A., Banks J. B. (2024). Further unpacking individual differences in mind wandering: the role of emotional valence and awareness. Conscious. Cogn. 122:103697. 10.1016/j.concog.2024.10369710.1016/j.concog.2024.103697PMC1125607138823316Wong Y. S., Willoughby A. R., Machado L. (2023). Reconceptualizing mind wandering from a switching perspective. Psychol. Res. 87, 357–372. 10.1007/s00426-022-01676-w10.1007/s00426-022-01676-wPMC992880235348846Yang T., Wu G. (2022). Spontaneous or deliberate: the dual influence of mind wandering on creative incubation. J. Creat. Behav. 56, 584–600. 10.1002/jocb.55310.1002/jocb.553