Young children use gestures to practice communicative functions that foster their receptive and expressive linguistic skills. Studies investigating the use of gestures by late talkers are limited. This study aimed to investigate the use of gestures and gesture-word combinations and their associations with word comprehension and word and sentence production in late talkers. A further purpose was to examine whether a set of individual and environmental factors accounted for interindividual differences in late talkers' gesture and gesture-word production. Sixty-one late talkers, including 35 full-term and 26 low-risk preterm children, participated in the study. Parents filled out the Italian short forms of the MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories (MB-CDI), "Gesture and Words" and "Words and Sentences" when their children were 30-months-old, and they were then invited to participate in a book-sharing session with their child. Children's gestures and words produced during the book-sharing session were transcribed and coded into CHAT of CHILDES and analyzed with CLAN. Types of spontaneous gestures (pointing and representational gestures) and gesture-word combinations (complementary, equivalent, and supplementary) were coded. Measures of word tokens and MLU were also computed. Correlational analyses documented that children's use of gesture-word combinations, particularly complementary and supplementary forms, in the book-sharing session was positively associated with linguistic skills both observed during the session (word tokens and MLU) and reported by parents (word comprehension, word production, and sentence production at the MB-CDI). Concerning individual factors, male gender was negatively associated with gesture and gesture-word use, as well as with MB-CDI action/gesture production. In contrast, having a low-risk preterm condition and being later-born were positively associated with the use of gestures and pointing gestures, and having a family history of language and/or learning disorders was positively associated with the use of representational gestures. Furthermore, a low-risk preterm status and a higher cognitive score were positively associated with gesture-word combinations, particularly complementary and supplementary types. With regard to environmental factors, older parental age was negatively associated with late talkers' use of gestures and pointing gestures. Interindividual differences in late talkers' gesture and gesture-word production were thus related to several intertwined individual and environmental factors. Among late talkers, use of gestures and gesture-word combinations represents a point of strength promoting receptive and expressive language acquisition.

Download full-text PDF

Source
http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC8997750PMC
http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19073918DOI Listing

Publication Analysis

Top Keywords

late talkers
20
gesture-word combinations
20
low-risk preterm
16
positively associated
16
gestures
12
gestures gesture-word
12
environmental factors
12
late talkers'
12
gesture gesture-word
12
book-sharing session
12

Similar Publications

Purpose: The purpose of this study was to determine if the Vocabulary Acquisition and Usage for Late Talkers (VAULT) intervention could be efficaciously applied to a new treatment target: words a child neither understood nor said. We also assessed whether the type of context variability used to encourage semantic learning (i.e.

View Article and Find Full Text PDF

Late Talkers' Language, Metaphor, Theory of Mind, and Reading Skills at 9 Years of Age.

J Speech Lang Hear Res

January 2025

Research Unit of Language and Communication, Department of Rehabilitation Sciences, TU Dortmund University, Germany.

Purpose: Prior work has found that "late talkers" (LTs) as a group continue to demonstrate lower language and reading outcomes compared to their typically developing (TD) peers even into young adulthood. Others identified that children diagnosed with developmental language disorder (DLD) show difficulties later with theory of mind (ToM) tasks and metaphor comprehension, but there is a shortage of research specifically investigating these advanced skills in LTs. The current study therefore compared language-related skills of former LTs with their TD peers at school age.

View Article and Find Full Text PDF

Purpose: To explore caregivers' experiences and engagement during the 16-week designed to support late talkers.

Method: Qualitative interpretative description methodology was used to understand the experience of five caregivers who had completed to support their children (aged 18-36 months). Caregivers attended a focus group to share their perspectives.

View Article and Find Full Text PDF

: Studies on night sleep and parental bedtime practices and their associations with language development in populations at risk of language delay and neonatal conditions, such as late talkers and preterm children, are scarce. : Our objective was to longitudinally examine the development of night sleep (total night sleep difficulties, settling, night waking, and co-sleeping), parental bedtime practices (total parental bedtime practices, active physical comforting, encouraging autonomy, and leaving to cry), and expressive language (word and sentence production), and their associations in low-risk preterm and full-term late talkers from 31 to 37 months of age. : Parents of 38 late talkers, 19 low-risk preterm and 19 full-term children, completed the Italian versions of the Infant Sleep Questionnaire, the Parental Interactive Bedtime Behavior Scale, and the MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventory Words and Sentences Long Form.

View Article and Find Full Text PDF

Purpose: The aim of the present study was twofold: to determine if deficits in motor skill proficiency and learning were present in 2-year-old children identified with early expressive language delay compared to peers without the delay, and to distinguish how motor skill proficiency and learning behaviors may manifest differently across culturally and linguistically diverse backgrounds.

Method: The study involved 54 children (24-36 months of age), 23 of whom were identified as having an expressive language delay. Furthermore, 16 participants were American and English-speaking and 38 were Israeli and Hebrew-speaking.

View Article and Find Full Text PDF

Want AI Summaries of new PubMed Abstracts delivered to your In-box?

Enter search terms and have AI summaries delivered each week - change queries or unsubscribe any time!