A sample of 62 untrained subjects were assessed on their ability to use unstructured numerical 9-point and 30-point category scales along with an unstructured line scale, using both rank-rating and serial monadic protocols. Visual stimuli were used for convenience, the task being to rate the heights of 12 easily discriminable columns of mung beans held in transparent vertical cylinders. Such stimuli had no perceptual variance, which would otherwise have added uncontrolled variance to the subjects' performance. Two measures of performance were used for each of the 6 experimental conditions. First, mean number of 'scaling errors' made in each of the six experimental conditions was computed. In this experiment, a scaling error was defined as giving a taller column a score equal to or less than a shorter column. The lower the error count, the better the subjects' performance. The second measure was to match the subjects' rating scale pattern of scores to a 'true' pattern of scores, derived from the physical measurements of the 12 columns. For this, a 'dissimilarity score' was developed. This compared the sum of the Euclidean distances between standardized true scale ratings for each of the column's 12 true heights, with those obtained from each subject. This gave a measure of the mismatch between the subject's set of scores and the true set of scores. Both the scaling error counts and the dissimilarity measures, indicated that subjects performed significantly better using the rank-rating protocol than the serial monadic. This was because of the effects of forgetting the exact intensities of stimuli once they had been removed, removal of stimuli being a necessary part of the serial monadic protocol. Subjects were penalized when using the 9-point scales, because there were too few categories to represent the different heights of all 12 columns. This introduced the concept of 'sufficient space'. Using the rank-rating protocol, the 30-point and line scales, with no memory problems and sufficient space elicited the best performances; they were not significantly different.
Download full-text PDF |
Source |
---|---|
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.foodres.2020.110027 | DOI Listing |
Prog Biophys Mol Biol
May 2024
University of California- Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA, USA. Electronic address:
Food Res Int
March 2021
Davis Sensory Institute, Davis, CA, USA; Department of Food Science & Technology, University of California, Davis, CA, USA. Electronic address:
A sample of 62 untrained subjects were assessed on their ability to use unstructured numerical 9-point and 30-point category scales along with an unstructured line scale, using both rank-rating and serial monadic protocols. Visual stimuli were used for convenience, the task being to rate the heights of 12 easily discriminable columns of mung beans held in transparent vertical cylinders. Such stimuli had no perceptual variance, which would otherwise have added uncontrolled variance to the subjects' performance.
View Article and Find Full Text PDFJ Sci Food Agric
August 2015
Department of Food Science & Technology, University of California, Davis, California, USA.
The 9-point hedonic scale has been used routinely in food science, the same way for 60 years. Now, with advances in technology, data from the scale are being used for more and more complex programs for statistical analysis and modeling. Accordingly, it is worth reconsidering the presentation protocols and the analyses associated with the scale, as well as some alternatives.
View Article and Find Full Text PDFThe Dyadic Mini Code (DMC) is designed to measure levels of synchrony in early infant-adult interaction. Based on important components of synchronous interaction reported in the literature, the instrument consists of six items, a total score, and a summary rating. Videotapes of 38 term and preterm mother-infant pairs were studied to establish reliability and validity of the tool.
View Article and Find Full Text PDFEnter search terms and have AI summaries delivered each week - change queries or unsubscribe any time!