Flavor-nutrient learning occurs when the post-ingestive consequences of a food are associated with its flavor. As a signal of the food's energy density, flavor-nutrient associations have the potential to contribute to the regulation of meal size. While all calorie sources (fat, carbohydrate, protein, ethanol) can support flavor-nutrient learning, prior research has found that flavor-nutrient associations based on fat may require higher nutrient concentrations and more rigorous experimental protocols than are required to train carbohydrate (cho)-based associations. To further explore potential macronutrient-specific differences in flavor-nutrient learning, the present study compared the time course of acquisition of cho- and fat-based associations. Rats were trained to associate distinctive flavors with high-density (3.2 kcal/mL) and low-density (0.2 kcal/mL) orally-consumed solutions, either fat (corn oil emulsion) or carbohydrate (sucrose). For each nutrient, both within- and between-group designs were used to assess (via two-bottle preference testing) whether flavor-nutrient learning had occurred after 2, 4, or 6 training trial pairs. Rats trained with carbohydrate demonstrated preferential intake of the low-density paired flavor after only 2 training pairs; in contrast, rats trained with fat required 6 training pairs. These findings demonstrate differential rapidity of acquisition flavor-nutrient associations. The longer time course of acquisition of fat-based flavor-nutrient associations may be yet another mechanism by which high-fat foods promote overeating.
Download full-text PDF |
Source |
---|---|
http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2692707 | PMC |
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2009.03.007 | DOI Listing |
PLoS Biol
December 2024
Champalimaud Research & Clinical Centre, Champalimaud Foundation, Av. de Brasília, Doca de Pedrouços, Lisboa, Portugal.
Postingestive nutrient stimulation conditions food preferences through striatal dopamine and may be associated with blunted brain responses in obesity. In a cross-sectional study, we tested flavor-nutrient conditioning (FNC) with maltodextrin-enriched yogurt, with maltodextrin previously optimized for concentration and dextrose equivalents (n = 57), and to mask texture cues (n = 102). After conditioning, healthy volunteers (n = 52) increased preference for maltodextrin-paired (+102 kcal, CS+), relative to control (+1.
View Article and Find Full Text PDFContemp Clin Trials
December 2024
Fralin Biomedical Research Institute at VTC, Roanoke, VA, USA; Center for Health Behaviors Research at Fralin Biomedical Research Institute at VTC, Roanoke, VA, USA; Department of Human Nutrition, Foods, and Exercise, Virginia Tech, Blacksburg, VA, USA. Electronic address:
Overconsumption of ultra-processed foods (UPFs), which are linked with adverse health outcomes, is a growing public health concern. UPFs deliver highly bioavailable calories rapidly, which may contribute to their reinforcing potential and drive overconsumption. Our primary aim is to test the role of speed of nutrient availability on reward learning.
View Article and Find Full Text PDFPhysiol Behav
October 2024
Departments of Psychiatry, New York University Grossman School of Medicine, 435 East 30th Street, New York, NY 10016, USA; Departments of Biochemistry and Molecular Pharmacology, New York University Grossman School of Medicine, 435 East 30th Street, New York, NY 10016, USA. Electronic address:
It was previously shown in striatal slices obtained from male rats that insulin excites cholinergic interneurons and increases dopamine (DA) release via α4β2 nicotinic receptors on DA terminals. The effect of insulin on DA release was blocked either by maintaining rats on a high sugar-high fat (HS-HF) diet that induced hyperinsulinemia and nucleus accumbens (NAc) insulin receptor insensitivity, or applying the α4β2 antagonist DHβE. In vivo, NAc shell insulin inactivation decreased a glucose lick microstructure parameter indicative of hedonic impact in male and female rats, and prevented flavor-nutrient learning, tested only in males.
View Article and Find Full Text PDFBehav Processes
August 2024
University of Granada, Department of Experimental Psychology, Centro de Investigación Mente, Cerebro y Comportamiento (CIMCYC), Spain. Electronic address:
Pairing a palatable flavor (US) with an initially neutral flavor cue (CS) results in an acquired conditioned preference for the latter. Two main associations have been proposed to explain the acquisition of flavor preferences: Flavor-Flavor and Flavor-Nutrient learning. Although the hedonic reaction triggered by US consumption has also been suggested as a possible additional component underlying acquired flavor preference, this issue has received little attention.
View Article and Find Full Text PDFChem Senses
January 2024
Department of Neuroscience & Behavior, Barnard College, Columbia University, 3009 Broadway, New York, NY 10027, United States.
This study examined how olfaction impacts ingestive responses of mice to sugar solutions. Experiment 1 asked whether naïve C57BL/6 (B6) mice could identify 1 M glucose, fructose, or sucrose solutions based on odor cues, during a 30-min 2-bottle acceptability test. We tested mice both before and after they were rendered anosmic with ZnSO4 treatment.
View Article and Find Full Text PDFEnter search terms and have AI summaries delivered each week - change queries or unsubscribe any time!