Within a broader programme developing murine models of Huntington's disease (HD), we have sought to develop a test of implicit learning for the mouse. Mice were trained in a novel serial visual discrimination task in the '9-hole box' operant test apparatus, followed by retesting after either bilateral quinolinic acid striatal lesions or sham lesions. In the task, each trial involves two sequential responses: an initial light stimulus is presented randomly in one of five holes, to which a nose-poke response results in the first light being extinguished and a second light is illuminated in a different hole. Response to the second light results in food reward, followed by a brief interval before the next trial. When the first light was in one of three of the five holes, the location of the second light was unpredictable in any of the remaining four holes; by contrast, if the first light occurred in one of the other two of the five holes, then the location of the second light was entirely predictable, being the hole two steps to the left or to the right, respectively. Reaction times and accuracy of responding were recorded to both stimuli. The mice learned the task with a degree of accuracy, and they demonstrated clear implicit learning, as measured by increased accuracy and reduced latency to respond to the presentation of the predictable stimulus. Striatal lesions disrupted performance, reducing accuracy for both the first and second stimuli and increasing response latencies for the second stimuli. The decrease in accuracy by the lesioned animals was accompanied by increases in perseverative nose-poking and inappropriate magazine entries throughout the trials, but the lesioned mice still showed a similar benefit (albeit, against a lower baseline of performance) from the implicit knowledge provided on predictable trials. The data validates the task as a sensitive probe for determining implicit learning deficits in the mouse, and suggests that the consequences of striatal lesions, while disrupting performance of skilled stimulus-response habits, are not selective to the process underlying implicit learning.
Download full-text PDF |
Source |
---|---|
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2004.11.021 | DOI Listing |
Atten Percept Psychophys
January 2025
Department of Psychology, The Ohio State University, 1835 Neil Ave, Columbus, OH, 43210, USA.
Our attention can sometimes be disrupted by salient but irrelevant objects in the environment. This distractor interference can be reduced when distractors appear frequently, allowing us to anticipate their presence. However, it remains unknown whether distractor frequency can be learned implicitly across distinct contexts.
View Article and Find Full Text PDFJ Neurophysiol
January 2025
KU Leuven, Department of Movement Sciences, B-3000 Leuven, Belgium.
In motor adaptation, learning is thought to rely on a combination of several processes. Two of these are implicit learning (incidental updating of the movement due to sensory prediction error) and explicit learning (intentional adjustment to reduce target error). The explicit component is thought to be fast adapting, while the implicit one is slow.
View Article and Find Full Text PDFPLoS One
January 2025
Neurorehabilitation and Biomechanics Research Section, Rehabilitation Medicine Department, Clinical Center, National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, MD, United States of America.
Children with cerebral palsy (CP) often participate in training to improve mobility, hand function and other motor abilities. However, responses to these interventions vary considerably across individuals even those with similar brain injuries, ages and functional levels. Dopamine is a neurotrasmitter known to affect motor skill acquistion in animals and humans and may be influenced by individual variations in genes related to brain transmission of dopamine.
View Article and Find Full Text PDFNat Rev Neurosci
January 2025
Intramural Research Program, National Institute on Drug Abuse, Baltimore, MD, USA.
Transient changes in the firing of midbrain dopamine neurons have been closely tied to the unidimensional value-based prediction error contained in temporal difference reinforcement learning models. However, whereas an abundance of work has now shown how well dopamine responses conform to the predictions of this hypothesis, far fewer studies have challenged its implicit assumption that dopamine is not involved in learning value-neutral features of reward. Here, we review studies in rats and humans that put this assumption to the test, and which suggest that dopamine transients provide a much richer signal that incorporates information that goes beyond integrated value.
View Article and Find Full Text PDFBioengineering (Basel)
December 2024
Institute of Biomedical Engineering, Department of Engineering Science, University of Oxford, Oxford OX3 7DQ, UK.
Cardiovascular diseases (CVDs) are the most common health threats worldwide. 2D X-ray invasive coronary angiography (ICA) remains the most widely adopted imaging modality for CVD assessment during real-time cardiac interventions. However, it is often difficult for the cardiologists to interpret the 3D geometry of coronary vessels based on 2D planes.
View Article and Find Full Text PDFEnter search terms and have AI summaries delivered each week - change queries or unsubscribe any time!