Question: How precisely can objects, located in different depth planes, be aligned to the same visual direction?

Methods: Twenty normal observers were presented with vertical Vernier lines at various stereodisparities. They had to judge whether the lower, anterior line was located on the right- or left-hand side of the upper, posterior line.

Results: Over a stereodisparity range from zero to 62'', the threshold for detecting a lateral offset between the Vernier lines remained at the "hyperacuity" level of about 7''. With larger stereodisparities, the threshold increased about fourfold, probably due to a mutual, partial suppression of the position signals from the right and left eyes. The reference point from which the observers judged the relative visual directions between stereodisparate objects was not located midway between the eyes; rather, it was often decentred towards the right or the left eye, meaning that the observers had an "ocular prevalence". Their ocular prevalence was, however, not strong enough to have an effect on the Vernier acuity for stereodisparate objects. (Under pathological conditions like strabismic amblyopia, one should expect a 100% prevalence of the good eye, implying that the Vernier acuity reaches the monocular level, irrespective of any depth difference between objects.)

Conclusion: Vernier acuity decreases with increasing stereodisparity. Ocular prevalence, occurring frequently among persons with normal eyes, has no effect on Vernier acuity for stereodisparate objects. For a typical everyday viewing condition, the reduced Vernier acuity beyond a stereodisparity of 62'' means that, from a viewing distance of 40 cm, precision mechanics have to guide their instrument as close as 0.4mm to a workpiece, until they can utilise their best position acuity.

Download full-text PDF

Source
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2004.09.045DOI Listing

Publication Analysis

Top Keywords

vernier acuity
24
stereodisparate objects
16
acuity stereodisparate
12
ocular prevalence
12
vernier
8
objects located
8
vernier lines
8
acuity
6
objects
5
stereodisparate
4

Similar Publications

The standard visual acuity measurements rely on stationary stimuli, either letters (Snellen charts), vertical lines (vernier acuity) or grating charts, processed by those regions of the visual system most sensitive to the stationary stimulation, receiving visual input from the central part of the visual field. Here, an acuity measurement is proposed based on discrimination of simple shapes, that are defined by motion of the dots in the random dot kinematograms (RDK) processed by visual regions sensitive to motion stimulation and receiving input also from the peripheral visual field. In the motion-acuity test, participants are asked to distinguish between a circle and an ellipse, with matching surfaces, built from RDKs, and separated from the background RDK either by coherence, direction, or velocity of dots.

View Article and Find Full Text PDF

Previous research has shown that neural activity in the primary visual cortex (V1) and V1 surface area may be linked with subjective experience of size illusions. Here, we behaviorally measured the hallway illusion with experimental manipulations as a proxy of V1's influence on size perception. We first tested whether the hallway illusion can persist without further recurrent processing by using backward masking.

View Article and Find Full Text PDF

Background: Keratoconus is associated with thinning and anterior protrusion of the cornea resulting in the symptoms of blurry and distorted vision. The commonly used clinical vision tests such as visual acuity and contrast sensitivity may not reflect the symptoms experienced in keratoconus and there are no quantitative tools to measure visual distortion. In this study, we used a quantitative test based on vernier alignment and field matching techniques to quantify visual distortion in keratoconus and assess its relation to corneal structural changes.

View Article and Find Full Text PDF

The relationship between mechanisms for curvature and Vernier detections.

Vision Res

May 2022

College of Comprehensive Psychology, Ritsumeikan University, 2-150, Iwakura-cho, Ibaraki-shi, Osaka 567-8570, Japan.

Tyler (Journal of Physiology 228 (1973) 637-647) reported that the sensitivity function for curvature detection has band-pass characteristics with respect to shape frequency. The shape of this function suggests that the curvature detection mechanism might consist of a set of sub-mechanisms each tuned for shape frequencies similar to mechanisms for contrast detection. In Experiment 1, we applied an adaptation for shape frequency to investigate whether shape frequency is the critical feature in curvature detection.

View Article and Find Full Text PDF

Want AI Summaries of new PubMed Abstracts delivered to your In-box?

Enter search terms and have AI summaries delivered each week - change queries or unsubscribe any time!