Laser-ablation inductively coupled-plasma mass-spectrometry analysis of red oak (Quercus rubra) from a well documented heavy metal contaminated United States Environmental Protection Agency superfund site in Woburn, Massachusetts reveals decade-long trends in Pb contaminant sources. Lead isotope ratios (207Pb/206Pb and 208Pb/206Pb) in tree rings plot along a linear trend bracketed by several local and regional contamination sources. Statistically significant interannual variations in 207Pb/206Pb suggest that atmospheric Pb is rapidly incorporated into wood, with minimal mobility subsequent to deposition in annual growth rings.
View Article and Find Full Text PDFThe discrimination of the depth of amplitude modulation of a signal carrier frequency can be disrupted by the presence of other modulated carriers (maskers), an effect called modulation discrimination interference (MDI). This paper examines whether MDI is influenced by the similarity in the envelope pattern of the signal and masker. A narrow-band noise (centered at 10 Hz) was used as the signal modulator.
View Article and Find Full Text PDFThe threshold for detecting 10-Hz amplitude modulation of a 2000-Hz carrier was measured in quiet, in the presence of an unmodulated masker, and in the presence of an amplitude-modulated masker. Two experiments were run; in each, the masker consisted of one or two sinusoidal carriers (chosen from among the frequencies of 800, 1600, 2400, and 3200 Hz). In experiment 1, the modulation rate of the masker ranged from 2 to 80 Hz.
View Article and Find Full Text PDFThresholds for detecting a temporal gap in a 20-Hz-wide band of noise (the target) were measured for the target alone, and in the presence of multiple 20-Hz-wide flanking bands presented to the opposite ear. The flanking bands caused gap thresholds to increase, and this effect was greater at higher levels of the flanking bands. The impairment to gap detection was greater when the flanking bands were comodulated with the target (i.
View Article and Find Full Text PDFThree subjects with unilateral cochlear hearing loss and three subjects with bilateral cochlear hearing loss were tested in three experiments. In the first, their auditory filter shapes were measured for center frequencies of 700 and 2000 Hz, using the notched-noise method. The auditory filters were generally broader for the impaired than for the normal ears.
View Article and Find Full Text PDFThe modulation depth required for the detection of sinusoidal amplitude modulation was measured as a function of modulation rate, giving temporal modulation transfer functions (TMTFs). The carrier was a one-octave wide noise centred at 2 kHz, and it was presented in an unmodulated background noise lowpass filtered at 5 kHz. Three subjects with unilateral cochlear hearing loss were tested.
View Article and Find Full Text PDFPhilos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci
June 1992
The detection of a change in the modulation pattern of a (target) carrier frequency, fc (for example a change in the depth of amplitude or frequency modulation, AM or FM) can be adversely affected by the presence of other modulated sounds (maskers) at frequencies remote from fc, an effect called modulation discrimination interference (MDI). MDI cannot be explained in terms of interaction of the sounds in the peripheral auditory system. It may result partly from a tendency for sounds which are modulated in a similar way to be perceptually 'grouped', i.
View Article and Find Full Text PDFThese experiments examine the effects of masker level on the magnitude of comodulation masking release (CMR). In experiment 1, threshold was measured for detecting a 2000-Hz signal in noise bands 100 or 3200 Hz wide, centered at the signal frequency. The noise was either amplitude modulated by a low-pass-filtered noise, or was unmodulated.
View Article and Find Full Text PDFAuditory filter shapes were derived from notched-noise masking data at center frequencies of 8 kHz (for three spectrum levels, N0 = 20, 35, and 50 dB) and 10 kHz (N0 = 50 dB). In order to minimize variability due to earphone placement, insert earphones (Etymotic Research ER2) were used and individual earmolds were made for each subject. These earphones were designed to give a flat frequency response at the eardrum for frequencies up to 14 kHz.
View Article and Find Full Text PDFJ Acoust Soc Am
April 1987
Psychometric functions were determined for the detection of temporal gaps in sinusoidal signals at center frequencies between 0.2 and 2.0 kHz.
View Article and Find Full Text PDFThresholds were measured for the detection of a temporal gap in a bandlimited noise signal presented in a continuous wideband masker, using an adaptive forced-choice procedure. In experiment I the ratio of signal spectrum level to masker spectrum level (the SMR) was fixed at 10 dB and gap thresholds were measured as a function of signal bandwidth at three center frequencies: 0.4, 1.
View Article and Find Full Text PDFJ Acoust Soc Am
February 1984
A two-interval, two-alternative forced choice task was used to estimate frequency difference limens (DLs) for individual harmonics within complex tones, and DLs for the periodicity (i.e., number of periods per s) of the whole complexes.
View Article and Find Full Text PDFThe threshold for detection of a temporal gap in a noiseband was measured. A notched noise masker was used to restrict listening to a limited spectral region. Threshold was measured as a function of center frequency, bandwidth, and level.
View Article and Find Full Text PDFThe level of noise required to mask tinnitus was measured in 12 subjects with sensorineural tinnitus; the noise was centred on the estimated pitch of the tinnitus, and the overall level required to just mask the tinnitus was measured for masker bandwidths of 2.5, 10, 20, 30 and 50% of the noise centre-frequency. For four of the tinnitus subjects, masking functions were similar to those observed in normal-hearing subjects with a "simulated" tinnitus (15-dB Sensation Level, 4-kHz pure tone).
View Article and Find Full Text PDF